Sunday, June 28, 2015

Daydreaming, mind wandering and their relations with cognitive abilities and mood.


Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual review of psychology, 66, 487-518.

We spend 25% to 50% of our waking hours engaged in thoughts that are not related to the here and now, or to the task we are doing at the moment.  Why does day dreaming occur?  What is its meaning?  What does it enable?

When we daydream, or when our mind wanders, our attention is driven away from the present stream of thought (and usually also from an external task) towards mental contents that are produced internally and not by the environment.  Often, thoughts that occur while our mind wanders are not related to  external stimuli  surrounding us at that moment.

It's possible to research daydreaming using four methods:

Probe-caught method – every now and then, randomly,  the participant  is asked about the content of his experience or about the thoughts he or she is currently thinking.

Self-caught method – the participant reports when he "catches" himself daydreaming.  Combining questioning and self reports enables us to estimate people's awareness of their thoughts.

Retrospective methoddata are gathered at the end of a task via questionnaires, preserving the natural time course of the task.

By an open method – at the end of a task,  the participant is asked o describe what she experienced during the task performance.

Through the open method, it was discovered that 48% of thoughts arising during daydreaming deal with the future, 29% deal with the present, 12% deal with the past and 11% cannot be located in time.  Studies show that dysphoria is related with more daydreaming about the past.  Daydreaming about the future tends to improve mood.

A study about people's ability to notice daydreams about an ex romantic partner of which they are trying not to think revealed  that people were "caught" (through the Self-caught method) daydreaming time and again about the ex partner, before they noticed it themselves.  The wish to be with that partner again was related to a higher chance of daydreaming about him and to a lower chance to be aware of it.

While performing different tasks, we turn our attention to the relevant stimuli and sensory modality and give them preference over stimuli and sensory modalities that are less relevant.  It is assumed, that when we daydream, we inhibit the processing of external perceptual information not related to the stream of thought that gets preference at that moment.  It's possible, that this inhibition results from the limited attentional resources that can't be turned inwards and outwards at the same time.

People with good executive control can limit the amount of mind wandering that is not related to the task when the task's demands are high.  There is a negative correlation between the frequency of mind wandering and  executive control ability during working memory tasks, sustained attention tasks and reading tasks.  People with good cognitive control tend to let their thoughts wander when they perform tasks that don't require a lot of attentional resources.  When task demands are low, executive control turns the freed attentional resoures to daydreaming.  This means that expertise in executive control is manifested in an ability to change the allocation of attentional resources to internal sources (daydreaming) or to external sources, in line with environmental demands.

People's tendency to daydream during tasks of working memory and intelligence tests predict  their SAT scores.  The more they tend  to daydream when performing these tasks the lower is their SAT score.  The ability to avoid daydreaming while performing a task that requires attentional resources is an important component of general intellectual ability. 

Daydreaming negatively impacts different cognitive functions, among them reading comprehension, and increases the risk of car accidents.  Training in mindfulness significantly decreases daydreaming while reading and performing working memory tasks, and helps improve performance in these tasks (mindfulness : a mental state entailing a primary focus on the circumstances of the present).

Training in monitoring our thoughts can help reduce daydreaming in inappropriate times.  Researchers think that mindfulness helps because it encourages people to pay attention to their thought contents, that is, to monitor their thoughts.  Taking  memory tests during a lecture also reduces daydreaming and improves  student's ability to remember the lecture's contents.

Daydreaming is related to creativity.  Writers were asked to write down creative ideas they've had during the day every evening for a fortnight, and to  note  the situation in which they occurred.   More than 40% of the creative ideas occurred when they were engaged in activities not related to work, or thought about something unrelated to the subject they were working on.  In another study, a relation was found between people's tendency to daydream and their performance on a test of Ideational Fluency (finding as many uses to a specific object as possible – a test measuring creativity and belonging to long term storage and retrieval ability).

It's possible that daydreaming provides mental breaks in task performance that enables us to rest during difficult task performance or to deal with boring tasks.    People  daydream more during routine and uncomplicated tasks.  For  example, pilots were especially likely to report mind wandering when engaging in flight segments that were going smoothly relative to segments in which they were having some difficulty.  When  pilots  took the role of copilot, mind wandering occurred nearly as twice as often as when they were in the role of the pilot.. 


No comments:

Post a Comment