Castles, A., & Nation, K. (2006). How does orthographic
learning happen? From inkmarks
to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing, 151. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.132.5739&rep=rep1&type=pdf
Word recognition (the ability to read
words automatically) develops with such remarkable speed that, by the end of
eighth grade, we expect children learning to read English to know and recognize
over 80,000 words!
How does this ability develop?
Beginning readers must acquire an alphabetic coding system,
that is, a mapping system between letters (graphems) and sounds
(phonemes). Phonological processing
ability (the ability to analyze and synthesize words into the sounds that
compose them) enables the acquisition of the alphabetic coding system. In order to read single words, however,
phonological awareness and an alphabetic coding system are not enough. One can't read the word "shoe", for
example, only using these systems. In order to read this and many
other words, we need specific orthographic knowledge.
It's possible to read single regular
English words (like "show") using phonological and alphabetic
skills. But these skills are not enough
for irregular word reading. In English,
the correlation between non-word reading (which is based on phonological and
alphabetic skills) and irregular word reading is lower than the correlation
between non-word reading and regular word reading. In order to read irregular words we need specific orthographic
knowledge.
What
is orthography?
Orthography is the system of signs
and rules used for graphic representation of language. Orthography includes letters and punctuation
marks, and the rules that describe how to use punctuation marks and how to
spell.
How
are orthographic knowledge and processing
assessed?
There are two methods. The first is orthographic choice, which measures the child's
ability to retrieve orthographic representations of specific words (which is a
word: Rain or Rane?). The second method
uses tasks that measure sensitivity to orthographic constrains and to general
orthographic characteristics of the language, beyond specific words (which
looks more like a word: FFEB or BEFF?).
One can argue that orthographic
choice tasks assess the
product of orthographic processing, and that orthographic sensitivity tasks
asses orthographic processing itself. But merely reading words and being exposed to
words improves and develops orthographic processing.
Even six year olds are sensitive to
the frequency and the legality of orthographic patterns, and even kindergarten children are
able to decide that a word string like PESS has a better chance to be a word
than a word string like PPES.
After merely 4 months of formal
schooling, first grade French children are sensitive to the fact that
consonants can be doubled in French only in medial position, not in initial or
final position. French children can also
distinguish between consonants that can and cannot be doubled in French.
Researchers use "print exposure"
as an indirect measure of exposure to orthographic patterns. "Print exposure" is the extent of
exposure or experience the child has with written material at home or in
kindergarten. Print exposure is measured
by asking the child which books he knows out of a list of popular children's
books. The list also contains names of
book that do not exist, in order to make sure that the child is not
guessing. Print exposure is correlated with orthographic skills
in 6-7 year olds. Print exposure has a
modest but significant contribution to word recognition beyond the contribution
of phonological and alphabetic skills.
A Matthew effect is operating
here. Early in the process of reading
acquisition, poor readers begin to be exposed to much less text than their
skilled reader friends. The texts that
poor readers are exposed to tend to be too hard for them, and thus the reading
experience tends to be unpleasant. As a
result these poor readers expose themselves to books even less, and the gap
between them and skilled readers widens.
Second grade orthographic processing
skills explain single word reading variation in third grade, beyond
phonological processing ability.
Is
there a relation between semantic skills and single word recognition?
In English, which has deep
orthography (there is no direct link between graphemes and phonemes. Words are not written as they sound),
children have to learn to read many irregular words. One way to do it is to use oral
vocabulary. The child partially decodes
a written word, and uses oral vocabulary to reach full decoding. Gradually, children with good vocabulary will develop better word
recognition skills. Semantic measures
like vocabulary, semantic fluency, familiarity with synonyms and listening
comprehension predict single word recognition in eight year olds, beyond
phonological skills. The relationship
between verbal-semantic skills and word reading is maintained over time, with
measures taken at 8 years predicting unique variance in word recognition some 4
years later when the children were 13 years old.
Some children have difficulty using
orthographic skills for single word recognition. These children have surface dyslexia. These children's alphabetic
decoding is intact. They can read
non-words at an average level, but they read real irregular words at a level
that is at least one standard deviation below average. The errors they make in irregular word
reading result from the use of alphabetic decoding rules. For example, they read "break" as
"breek" or "shoe" as "show". They have difficulty with orthographic choice
tasks (which is a vegetable: BEEN or BEAN?).
These children show no signs of semantic or vocabulary
difficulties. They don't have visual
disabilities or poor visual memory. They
simply did not acquire specific orthographic word reading skills.
Israeli Professor David Share
developed a theory according to which orthography is self
taught. His studies that support
the theory look like this: eight year
olds read short stories, each containing one new word, which is repeated four
to six times in the story (a non-word, for example the name of a fictional
place – "Yate"). After three
days, orthographic learning of the new word is assessed. The word "Yate" is presented with a
homophone like "Yait" and non-homophonic foils that contain the same
letters, like "Taye". The
child has to recognize the word that he read.
Children recognize the right word 70% of the times. They can also read the right words faster
than the foils, and tend to spell them correctly. Thus, there is clear evidence for orthographic learning in eight year
olds, even after only four exposures to a novel word. Sometimes even one exposure is enough!
However, one or four exposures to
words don't cause orthographic learning at the beginning of first grade. Hebrew orthography (with diacritical marks)
is transparent (the relation between grapheme to phoneme is straightforward). Share argues that transparent orthography
encourages a reading pattern that is similar to surface dyslexia. Children at the process of Hebrew reading acquisition
lean on the alphabetic system. Only
after exposure to a significant number of words they begin to develop
orthographic sensitivity. Share argued
that children learning to read English teach themselves orthography at an
earlier age than children learning to read Hebrew.
It's reasonable to assume that in any
point in time, a child may read specific words slowly and laboriously, leaning heavily
on context and alphabetic coding, while reading other words quickly,
automatically and with a minimal contextual influence.
How
does orthographic learning happen? Does
the child generalize orthographic rules out of words he is exposed to?
In French, some consonants can be duplicated, but the letter K is never duplicated (there are no
French words that contain KK). Vowels
are never duplicated in French. French
children at the beginning of first grade think that non-words like TUKKE
"look more like a word" than non-words like TUUKE. Does this mean that they generalize the rule
"you can duplicate consonants but can't duplicate vowels"?
Apparently,
orthographic learning is more similar to statistical learning. Children learn which specific consonants are duplicated in
French and which are not, or rather which consonants have a better or a lesser chance to be
duplicated.
First grade French children think
that the non-word BUKKOX "looks more like a word" than the non-word
"BUKOXX", despite the fact that both KK and XX don't exist in
French. Apparently, this means that the
children apply the rule "a consonant is duplicated only in the middle of a
word and not at the end of it". But
these children succeeded even better to judge that the non-word GOLLIR
"looks more like a word" than the non-word GOLIRR. The letter strings RR and LL do appear in
French. If the children were only using
the rule "a consonant is multiplied only in the middle of a word"
there would have been no difference in success between the pair GOLLIR/GOLIRR
and BUKKOX/BUKOXX. The fact that there
is a difference suggests that children don't only apply this rule, but are also
aware of the statistical
probability of doubling LL and doubling KK.
No comments:
Post a Comment