Blashfield, R. K., Keeley, J. W., Flanagan, E. H., & Miles, S. R. (2014). The Cycle of Classification: DSM-I Through DSM-5. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol, 10, 25-51.
The struggles and controversies surrounding
mental disorder definitions in the various DSMs remind me of the struggles surrounding
learning disability definition. This is
one of the reasons it's good to know them.
DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders
This paper reviews DSM development. The paper
uses the term "patients" to describe people in need of psychiatric intervention
and, thus, DSM definitions. I'll use the
term "patients" in this post following this paper. The way we name or refer to our customers
reflects a value judgement, to my opinion (I prefer to call them "clients"). This is also the case with the way we name our
clients' problems (this paper names them "mental disorders").
Here are some interesting points from
this paper.
We begin with this fascinating table:
NAME
|
PUBLICATION YEAR
|
NUMBER OF PAGES
|
NUMBER OF DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES
|
PRICE IN DOLLARS
|
REVENUE FOR THE AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC
ASSOCIATION IN MILLIONS OF DOLLARS
|
DSM1
|
1952
|
132
|
128
|
3$
|
UNKNOWN
|
DSM2
|
1968
|
119
|
193
|
3.5$
|
1.27
|
DSM3
|
1980
|
494
|
228
|
31.75$
|
9.33
|
DSM3R
|
1987
|
567
|
253
|
NOT WRITTEN IN THE PAPER
|
16.65
|
DSM4
|
1994
|
886
|
383
|
$48.95
|
120
|
DSM4TR
|
2000
|
943
|
383
|
$74.95
|
UNKNOWN
|
DSM5
|
2013
|
947
|
541
|
$199
|
UNKNOWN
|
DSM1
Following the Second World War, there
were four classification systems of mental disorders in the US. The
American Psychiatric Association (APA) decided to overcome this “Tower of
Babel” situation by creating a classification that would be acceptable to all
members of its organization and that could unify the diagnostic terms of its
psychiatrists. The result was the DSM1. DSM-I
had a hierarchical system in which the initial node in the hierarchy was
differentiating organic brain syndromes from “functional” disorders. The
functional disorders were further subdivided into psychotic versus neurotic
versus character disorders. This organization roughly followed the
decision-making process of clinicians.
The
DSM-I descriptions of disorders were prose paragraphs that incorporated
behavioral and trait-like criteria. The terms in the description were relative
and left to the interpretation of the clinician, leading to problems with
reliability across professionals.
DSM2
DSM2 published in 1968 was the result of
an effort to unite the classification systems in the world. It was organized in a similar way as the DSM1. Many of the new categories added in the
DSM-II were categories of relevance to outpatient mental health efforts.
In 1971 Kendell et al conducted a study
in which a set of eight videotapes of patients from the United States and Great
Britain were shown to groups of American and British psychiatrists. For all
eight videotapes, the modal diagnosis by the American clinicians was
schizophrenia. In contrast, some of the videotapes, in the opinion of the
British psychiatrists, represented patients with manic-depressive disorders,
schizophrenia, and personality disorders. The results, that showed there are still
differences in diagnoses between professionals in different countries, were
considered as evidence that Americans tended to be over inclusive in their use
of schizophrenia as a diagnosis.
In 1973 Rosenhan published a provocative
paper in Science about how a group of colleagues went to different inpatient
facilities in the United States requesting admission. They were truthful about
themselves during the intake interview except for two things: (a) they gave
fictitious names so that their admissions would not appear on their future
medical records, and (b) they reported hearing a voice saying “Empty” or
“Thud.” All were admitted with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Their average
length of stay in the inpatient facility was nineteen days (the total range was
7 to 52 days). When discharged, most of them were given a diagnosis of
“schizophrenia, in remission.” Rosenhan and his colleagues noted that most of
the patients in the facilities spotted that they were fakes, but none of the
pseudopatients were detected by the hospital staff. Rosenhan concluded that
inpatient facilities of the time could not differentiate the sane from the
insane. Rosenhan’s paper stirred up a
firestorm of reactions
.
These and other studies were stimuli for
changes in the DSM3.
DSM3
Publishing the DSM-III in 1980 was part
of a paradigm shift in psychiatry (and the mental health field in general).
Prior to the DSM-III, psychiatry was dominated by psychoanalytically trained
psychiatrists. These psychoanalysts saw little value to clinical diagnosis for
working with psychotherapy patients. In contrast, the main authors of the
DSM-III attempted to bring psychiatry back to its medical roots. Their ideas fit
well with the transition in treatment focus from psychotherapy to the use of
medications.
The DSM3 authors will to drop the term
"neurosis" from the DSM raised a lot of controversy among
professionals. A compromised was reached
in which the word "neurosis" appeared in parenthesis following the
word "disorder" in specific cases.
Robert Spitzer, who was the head of the
DSM-III and the organizing committee for the DSM-III took the bold step of
proposing a tentative definition of the concept of mental disorder. They needed
this definition because an explicit goal of the creators of the DSM-III was to
avoid speculations about the causal mechanisms (especially theoretical concepts
couched in psychoanalytic terms) that explained psychopathology. This
definition was also in direct contrast to the antipsychiatry movement that
attempted to define a mental disorder as society’s way of dealing with
undesirable people—by labeling them with a mental disorder to keep them quiet
and segregated.
The definition in the DSM-III was: Each
of the mental disorders is conceptualized as a clinically significant
behavioral or psychological syndrome or pattern that occurs in an individual
and that is typically associated with either a painful symptom (distress) or
impairment in one or more important areas of functioning (disability). In
addition, there is an inference that there is a behavioral, psychological, or
biological dysfunction, and that the disturbance is not only in the
relationship between the individual and the society.
The DSM-III definition of mental disorder
led to an interesting and growing discussion of psychiatric classification by
philosophers, cognitive psychologists, social anthropologists, and historians.
The DSM-III contained diagnostic criteria
to specify the meaning of the categories. In addition, for each category, there
was a description of the typical demographic profile of patients experiencing
this disorder, a lengthy prose explanation of what the category meant, a
description of how to differentiate the target category from any other category
with which it might be confused, and a brief discussion of what was known, if
anything, about the course and onset of the disorder. Another innovation to the
DSM-III was that the system was multiaxial. Each patient was expected to be
diagnosed along five separate axes: (a) the descriptive presentation of the
patient (i.e., the mental disorder categories), (b) the underlying personality
and/or intellectual disorder, (c) any associated medical disorder that was
relevant to the patient’s psychiatric presentation, (d ) the psychosocial
stressors in the patient’s environment, and (e) the patient’s highest level of
adaptive functioning in the past year.
After
the publication of the DSM-III, Spitzer and his colleagues created the SCID
(Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R). By the
year 2000 there were over 240 instruments to measure various aspects of
psychopathology and mental disorders. The
reliability of diagnostic assessment using these new instruments generally was
a distinct improvement over what had been found in the pre-DSM-III research.
Spitzer & Fleiss’s (1974) review of pre-DSM-III reliability research showed
estimates of interclinician agreement typically ranging from 0.4 to 0.6. Using
structured interviews like the SCID, reliability estimates were distinctly
higher, typically in the range of 0.75 to 0.90. Because of the clearly defined
method for assigning psychopathology, along with improved reliability, structured
interviews would soon dominate the research world although even today they are
rarely used in clinical practice.
How reflective was the DSM3 structure of
the "natural" way clinicians conducted diagnoses?
Cantor et al. (1980) had 13 mental health
clinicians list the features that they associated with nine DSM-II diagnostic
categories of psychosis. Any feature that was chosen by at least 3 of the 13
clinicians was kept for the final feature list. Then they took twelve case
histories of patients that had been given one of four psychotic diagnoses
(manic, depressed, paranoid schizophrenia, and undifferentiated schizophrenia).
Four cases were considered to be quite prototypical of the four diagnoses
(i.e., these four cases contain almost all of the features generated by the 13
clinicians), four were moderately prototypical, and four were not typical
(i.e., these four cases had four or less of the defining features generated by
the 13 clinicians). These case histories were given to the clinicians to
diagnose. The reliability of the diagnoses varied as a function of the
prototypicality of the case histories, with the least prototypical cases having
the lowest reliability. This research and
others suggested that clinicians did not
use diagnostic criteria to make diagnoses. Clinicians’ diagnoses tended to follow a
prototype-matching model rather than a criteria-based model.
DSM3R
DSM3R that was published in 1987 was not
structurally different than the DSM3 (it had the axis system, used diagnostic
criteria and the mental disorders were organized in a similar way) but it
contained new categories.
The political struggles concerning the
DSM-III centered around a battle between a psychoanalytic faction of the APA
and a biologically-oriented faction. When the DSM-III-R was being created, the
focus of controversy shifted. Feminists were concerned with proposals by the
DSM-III-R committees for new categories such as premenstrual syndrome and
masochistic PD. As a result of the controversy, the DSM-III-R added a new
appendix to its classifications called “Proposed diagnostic categories needing
further study.” Contained in this appendix were three categories: late luteal
phase dysphoric disorder (the new name for premenstrual syndrome), sadistic PD
(to balance masochistic PD), and self-defeating PD (the new name for
masochistic PD).
In 1992, WAKEFIELD raised attention the
fact that a value judgment is necessary to instantiate any definition of mental
disorder. The same symptoms might be judged as
disordered in one context but not in another. Further, as societal and
individual values change over time, some conditions that used to be disordered
will no longer be considered abnormal (e.g., homosexuality), and others that
were not disordered might become problematic (e.g., Internet use). Thus, there
can never be a “final” version of the DSM.
To be continued in a following post…
No comments:
Post a Comment