A child can be diagnosed as learning disabled when none of
the following factors is the main reason for his reading/writing/math
difficulties: sensory impairment, intellectual disability, emotional or psychological disturbances,
other neurological disorders, lack of motivation, fatigue, poor or ineffective
instruction, cultural differences, linguistic differences (i.e., limited
English proficiency), economic disadvantage or psychosocial adversity.
Thus, if
we are under the impression that the child's difficulties in reading, writing or arithmetic are mainly due to economic
disadvantage or psychosocial adversity, we cannot diagnose him as learning
disabled.
Still
we'd like to give this child support in school, but we don't want to assign him
the learning disability label if this is not the main reason for his
difficulties.
How can an
economic disadvantage be the main cause of reading, writing or arithmetic
difficulties?
One way is through the influence of comprehension knowledge
on reading, writing and arithmetic. A
child with limited vocabulary and general information will have a hard time
comprehending what he reads (since he may not know key words in the text and
may not be familiar with the subject the text deals with). This child may also have trouble expressing
himself richly and with complexity in
written language. The breadth of
comprehension knowledge also influences arithmetic. A child with poor comprehension knowledge may
have difficulties understanding word problems and may have poor
"arithmetic vocabulary" (words like "denominator, power etc.).
When
researchers looked at the effect of daily interactions between parents and
children on language and vocabulary development, they found that up to the age
of three, children from high SES (socioeconomic status) families were exposed
to 30 million more words than children from very low SES families! This gap was widened with age!
I assume that
due to differences in exposure, there may be differences between children from
high SES and low SES families in understanding and using syntactical and
grammatical structures typical of high register English.
Children from
low SES families have lesser chances to visit museums or go on trips. They may
have less books at home and have less extracurricular activities. This lack of experience may affect their
comprehension knowledge.
Obviously
these are generalizations that are not true for every child from low SES
background. Clearly some children raised
in low SES backgrounds have books at home and are avid readers. Some children raised in low SES backgrounds
have enriching conversations with their parents. We should consider the situation of the
specific child we work with and not judge him by the group he belongs to. But generally, this tendency does exist.
Can low
SES adversely affect other cognitive abilities, beside comprehension knowledge?
Items in intelligence tests are derived
from the wealth of knowledge and skills of the test's developers, who are
usually people from average or higher SES background, well acculturated to the
dominant culture in which they live.
One evidence
of the links between SES and general
intelligence can be found in the WISC-R Hebrew manual (translated by
me): "the degree of nurturance given to the child in the environment he is
raised in is…another possible factor influencing his raw scores…There is no
doubt that in this respect there are marked differences among scores of
children of the same age and the same school experience – differences in
learning opportunities and extracurricular activities, differences in the
degree of support and encouragement these children receive and so on. Psychologists agree about the unique effect
of environmental nurturance on the development of children. Hence, among children of the same age and the
same school experience, the average raw score in a test will rise as the level
of environmental nurturance rises… This is the reason
that the estimate of the real intellectual potential of the child is biased: the intellectual potential of children raised
in a nurturing environment is overestimated, and the real potential of children
raised in a deprived environment is underestimated…In order to correct for this
bias…one has to use separate norms for different levels of nurturance…[But it's
difficult] to separate the unique effect of nurturance on raw test scores.
We decided…to
assume the differences in nurturance cause about half of the differences among
raw scores of children at the same age having the same amount of school
experience…We recommend using a "rule of thumb" for a crude correction of the IQ scores derived from the
norm tables, when they are especially biased, that is, when it's clear that the
child was raised in an extreme environment in respect of nurturance. For children raised in an especially deprived environment, we recommend
adding 7 points to the measured FSIQ (full scale IQ), and from the scores of
children raised in an especially nurturing environment we recommend subtracting
7 points."
I hope to
present in future posts more evidence and possible effects of SES on specific
cognitive abilities.
What is the significance of this for diagnosing
children raised in "an especially deprived environment" as learning
disabled?
We don't
want to diagnose a child whose main cause for low achievement is environmental
conditions related to his SES as learning disabled. On the other hand, it seems that an
especially deprived environment affects FSIQ.
As a result, a child from low SES may have a few low cognitive abilities. These abilities have probably been adversely affected
by SES and not by disability. We don't
want to diagnose this child as a "slow learner" either. We still want very much to provide him with assistance
in school.
I don't
know about other countries, but in Israel a child has to have a disability of
some sort in order to get assistance in a regular school. That's how poor children slip between the
cracks.
References (some are in Hebrew):
1. Colker, Ruth. The learning disability mess.
Journal of gender, social policy and the law, 2011, 20(81-106)
3. Flanagan, D.
P., Ortiz, S. O., Alfonso, V. C., & Dynda, A. M. (2006). Integration of
response to intervention and norm‐referenced tests in learning disability
identification: Learning from the Tower of Babel. Psychology in the Schools, 43(7), 807-825.
4. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual
of mental disorders (5th
ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing
5. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early
catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by
age 3. American educator, 27(1), 4-9.
6. McGrew, K. S., &
Wendling, B. J. (2010). Cattell–Horn–Carroll cognitive‐achievement relations: What we have learned from the past 20
years of research. Psychology
in the Schools, 47(7),
651-675. http://www.iapsych.com/kmpubs/mcgrew2010.pdf
7. קאהן, ס. מדריך למבחן האינטליגנציה WISCR95. משרד
החינוך, התרבות והספורט – השירות הפסיכולוגי – ייעוצי. מכון הנרייטה סאלד
למחקר במדעי ההתנהגות. 1998.
No comments:
Post a Comment