ברוכים הבאים! בלוג זה נועד לספק משאבים לפסיכולוגים חינוכיים ואחרים בנושאים הקשורים לדיאגנוסטיקה באורייטנצית CHC אבל לא רק.

בבלוג יוצגו מאמרים נבחרים וכן מצגות שלי וחומרים נוספים.

אם אתם חדשים כאן, אני ממליצה לכם לעיין בסדרת המצגות המופיעה בטור הימני, שכותרתה "משכל ויכולות קוגניטיביות".

Welcome! This blog is intended to provide assessment resources for Educational and other psychologists.

The material is CHC - oriented , but not entirely so.

The blog features selected papers, presentations made by me and other materials.

If you're new here, I suggest reading the presentation series in the right hand column – "intelligence and cognitive abilities".

נהנית מהבלוג? למה שלא תעקוב/תעקבי אחרי?

Enjoy this blog? Become a follower!

Followers

Search This Blog

Featured Post

קובץ פוסטים על מבחן הוודקוק

      רוצים לדעת יותר על מבחן הוודקוק? לנוחותכם ריכזתי כאן קובץ פוסטים שעוסקים במבחן:   1.      קשרים בין יכולות קוגניטיביות במבחן ה...

Saturday, May 28, 2016

The Homophone Meaning Generation Test

  

When people are asked to supply as many meanings as they can for a word presented orally and out of context, they begin with the most frequent meanings, and once these are exhausted, turn to other, less common, meanings.  This process requires a strategic search in the knowledge base and mental flexibility, which are part of executive functions.

The homophone meaning generation test – HMGT , developed in Hebrew by Prof. Gitit Kave and her colleagues,   tests this process.  Homophones are words which have different meanings that "sound the same".  For instance, the word "right" has the meaning of "a direction (opposed to "left")" and the meaning of "appropriate, suitable".  

The test in its Hebrew form is made of 24 homophones, each having between three and ten possible meanings.  Half of the words are also homographs (all their meanings are spelled  the same way).  The words are presented orally, one by one.  The person is asked to think about as many meanings as he can to each word.  There is no time limit.  When the person is not able to think about any more meanings, the next word is presented.  Each different meaning is assigned a point.  The score in the test is the number of meanings given to all presented words.  The production of meanings requires strategic search and mental flexibility, thus the test is supposed to measure executive functions. 

Prof. Kave and her colleagues performed a few experiments with this test, in children and adults, two of them will be presented here, 

In the adult study, the relation between the homophone test and shifting and clustering in fluency tests was examined.

What are shifting and clustering?

Phonemic fluency tests (in which the person is asked to name as many words beginning with a specific letter within one minute as he can) and semantic fluency tests (in which a person is asked to say as many words belonging to a specific category within one minute as he can) measure the ability to search and retrieve information from long term memory (thus they belong to the broad ability "Long Term Storage and Retrieval").

In both of these tests, the memory search includes two aspects:  clusters and switches.  During test performance people usually produce clusters of words that are related to each other semantically (for instance, "horse, cow, donkey, goat, sheep" is a cluster of "farm animals") or phonetically (for example, "buy, bug, bus" is a cluster of words beginning with "bu").  When the subcategory from which the words were retrieved is exhausted (the person cannot think about more farm animals or more words beginning with "bu"), the person shifts to another subcategory (for instance, he will transfer to "sea animals" or to "be").

Clusters reflect the semantic organization of the crystallized knowledge.  Shifts reflect executive functions (strategic search in the fund of lexical knowledge, monitoring, the initiation of response, flexibility, an ability to shift set).

The phonemic fluency test is presumed to require executive functions more than the semantic fluency test.  This is because when we retrieve information by content (like in the semantic fluency test) we perform a familiar act which fits the way our knowledge is organized.  But when we retrieve words beginning with a specific letter, we perform a new and unfamiliar task.  In order to optimally perform this task we have to come up with a search strategy, monitor the search process and act flexibly.  These are executive functions.

One hundred volunteers aged 18-35 and with an average of 13.8 years of  education participated in the study.  All participants were healthy native Hebrew speakers.  Each participant was tested with the homophone test and the semantic and phonemic fluency tests.

Since the homophone test was built as an executive functioning test, requiring directed and flexible memory search, the assumption was that the homophone test will be correlated with phonemic fluency more than with semantic fluency.  In fact, a significant and equal correlation was found between the homophone test and both fluency tests.

The authors offer 2 kinds of explanations for this finding:  it may be that the lexical knowledge component existing in the homophone test and the semantic fluency test caused the correlation between these tests to be higher than expected.  Another possibility is that both fluency tests require a similar executive search.  The relation between all three tests can be resulting from a common executive component, which is necessary for successful performance in these tasks.

When we look at the shifts and clusters data we get a finer picture:

Performance in the homophone test was related more to the number of switches or the number of clusters that a person performed in both fluency tests, than to the average size of the cluster in these tests.  This strengthens the hypothesis, that the relation between the three tests is stronger in the executive component than in the lexical semantic component.  While members in a sematic cluster (for example, "dog, cat, hare, gerbil") belong to a similar conceptual field, this is not the case with homophone representations.    Thus performance in the homophone test cannot be attributed to the spread of activation within a sub category in the semantic lexicon 

Participants in this study produced more meanings for non-homographic homophones than for words that are both homophones and homographs.  The score of the homographs was more strongly related to the switching component in both fluency tests than the score on the non homographs.  This means that finding different meanings for homophones that are also homographs requires more executive functions than finding different meanings for homophones that are not homographs. 


How do children perform in the homophone test?  A child cannot retrieve meanings that do not yet exist in his lexicon.  But sometimes a child will find it hard to retrieve meanings he is familiar with since his retrieval skills may not be flexible enough.  Children's retrieval skills are affected by the development of their vocabulary and the development of controlled search processes in their existing lexical knowledge. 

Children's vocabulary is developing constantly, as the child is exposed to reading and literature.  At the end of 2nd grade, English speaking children's vocabulary contains 6000 word meanings.  Their vocabulary increases in the next few years at a rate of 1000 meanings per year.  In addition to the growing lexicon, knowledge about word meanings also grows.  The meanings of new words are gradually refined in a process that continues into adolescence.

Efficient  search strategies also develop throughout childhood and into adolescenceThese search strategies are one of the manifestations of executive functions. 

In the children's study, changes in word retrieval throughout childhood were assessed by four tasks:  a picture naming task, a phonemic fluency task, a semantic fluency task and the homophone test.  These tasks differ in the amount of flexibility needed to perform the test. 

In the picture naming task (in which the child sees a picture of an object and is asked to name it) there is no need for flexibility in lexical search.  Each picture corresponds to one word only.  When this word is found, the search is over.

Semantic and phonemic fluency tests require a more flexible search in the lexicon, using clustering and switching strategies as was discussed earlier.  There is a developmental improvement in the performance on these tests up to the age of 12 and beyond.  The fluency component that improves the most throughout childhood is the shifting component.  As was said before, this is an executive component which reflects strategic search, response initiation, monitoring and flexibility.

In order to perform the homophone test optimally, the word search has to continue much after the first word has been retrieved.  Unlike the fluency tests in which the search is performed on a limited set of the lexical knowledge, the homophone test requires searching and retrieving from the whole lexicon.  Thus it may require a more flexible search strategy than is required for the other tests.

Two hundred and seven children participated in the study, 20 from each age group in the age range of 8-17 and in grades 3rd to 12th.  The children were born in Israel and speak Hebrew as a first language.  They don't have learning disabilities, neurological or developmental problems and their socioeconomic status is average.

The children's scores on all four tests rose steadily between age 8 and age 17.  Picture naming ability rose the least (although the test being used was probably too easy in its Hebrew version, since the youngest children succeeded in 83% of its items).    Scores on the homophone test showed the steepest rise.  Thus, performance on the homophone test changed the most during development.

Like the adults, children produced less meanings for homophones that are also homographs (all their meanings are spelled the same) than for homophones that are not homographs.  The rate of development in performance on the homographs was identical to the rate of development in performance on non homographs. 

To summarize, this test assesses executive functions in a verbal task.  High scores on the homophone test indicate that the person has a rich lexical network and that he is able to shift between meanings flexibly.

How would this test be classified in CHC abilities?

I've recently written about the place of executive functions in CHC theory – "between" fluid ability and short term memory. 
  
The homophone test does not fit any of the definitions of the Fluid  narrow abilities (induction, deduction, quantitative reasoning).  In Hoelzle's study, other tests measuring executive functions are classified under fluid ability.  Hoelzle did not classify the homophone test or similar tests.  Due to the nature of this test and the classification of other executive function tests to fluid ability, perhaps we can classify this test also to fluid ability, at least tentatively.

The homophone test does not fit any of the definitions of narrow abilities within Short Term Memory.

The narrow ability "Ideational fluency" (in "Long Term Storage and Retrieval") is defined as the "ability to rapidly produce a series of ideas, words, or phrases related to a specific condition or object.".  Because of the speed factor, the homophone test cannot be classified here.

The narrow ability "Lexical knowledge" (in "Comprehension  - Knowledge") is "knowledge of the definitions of words and the concepts that underlie them".  The homophone test does require such knowledge and seems to fit here.

So it seems that in CHC terms the test can be considered to test Fluid ability and Comprehension Knowledge (Lexical Knowledge).

References:


KavÉ, G., Avraham, A., Kukulansky-Segal, D., & Herzberg, O. (2007). How does the homophone meaning generation test associate with the phonemic and semantic fluency tests? A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society13(03), 424-432.  

Kavé, G., Kukulansky-Segal, D., Avraham, A., Herzberg, O., & Landa, J. (2010). Searching for the right word: Performance on four word-retrieval tasks across childhood. Child Neuropsychology16(6), 549-563.


Hoelzle, J. B. (2008). Neuropsychological assessment and the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities model. ProQuest.    PAGE 114   http://utdr.utoledo.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2213&context=theses-dissertations

No comments:

Post a Comment