ברוכים הבאים! בלוג זה נועד לספק משאבים לפסיכולוגים חינוכיים ואחרים בנושאים הקשורים לדיאגנוסטיקה באורייטנצית CHC אבל לא רק.

בבלוג יוצגו מאמרים נבחרים וכן מצגות שלי וחומרים נוספים.

אם אתם חדשים כאן, אני ממליצה לכם לעיין בסדרת המצגות המופיעה בטור הימני, שכותרתה "משכל ויכולות קוגניטיביות".

Welcome! This blog is intended to provide assessment resources for Educational and other psychologists.

The material is CHC - oriented , but not entirely so.

The blog features selected papers, presentations made by me and other materials.

If you're new here, I suggest reading the presentation series in the right hand column – "intelligence and cognitive abilities".

נהנית מהבלוג? למה שלא תעקוב/תעקבי אחרי?

Enjoy this blog? Become a follower!

Followers

Search This Blog

Featured Post

קובץ פוסטים על מבחן הוודקוק

      רוצים לדעת יותר על מבחן הוודקוק? לנוחותכם ריכזתי כאן קובץ פוסטים שעוסקים במבחן:   1.      קשרים בין יכולות קוגניטיביות במבחן ה...

Saturday, October 15, 2016

Is episodic memory a (narrow) cognitive ability? Part three: Individual differences in episodic memory – the case of especially poor episodic memory

 .
Can episodic memory be a (narrow) cognitive ability?  It has to fulfill a number of conditions for that.  In the first post of this series we've seen that episodic memory develops through the lifetime.  In the second post we've began to look into individual differences in episodic memory.  We've discussed people with especially high episodic memory.

In this post we'll discuss a group of people with an especially poor episodic memory. 

First, a quick reminder of concepts (you can skip to the green color below):

Episodic memory is memory for life events that we'd experienced in a specific place at a specific time.  The hallmark of episodic memory is the experience of mentally "being there".

 Autobiographical memory is composed of episodic memory and semantic knowledge about ourselves and about things that took place in the world during our lifetimes. Semantic knowledge about the self is, for example, knowledge about our character and hobbies and about places we've been to, in a way that is not necessarily tied with specific episodic memories (I know I'd visited a specific place and know what it looks like, but don't remember the experience itself).

Semantic memory is the fund of knowledge we've accumulated over the years (facts, ideas, word and concept meanings, knowledge about our culture).

In 2004 a woman named Susan McKinnon read a paper about psychologist Endel Tulving, that drew the distinction between semantic and episodic memory.  The paper presented a case of a man whose episodic memory was negatively affected as a result of brain damage following a car accident. McKinnon realized that that man's symptoms resemble her experiences (minus the brain damage).

Tulving expressed his belief that there are intelligent and completely healthy people with no ability to remember personal experiences.  These people have poor episodic memory.  Tulving thought that soon these people will be discovered.  McKinnon realized Tulving is referring to people like her. 

McKinnon is healthy, she is married and has a family.  She is now retired and in the past has worked as a professional.  She doesn't remember any events of her life.  She doesn't remember good and exciting events, arguments or traumatic events.  She knows about the existence of these events but has no experience of them.  She doesn't remember books she'd read or movies or TV series she'd watched.  Her ability to form mental images is not very good.

In 2006 McKinnon decided to approached Dr. Brian Levine who worked with Tulving.  Levine tested her and she became the first person to be diagnosed with SDAM – severely deficient autobiographical memory.  Later Levine found two more people with a similar autobiographical memory, and in 2015 published with his colleagues Palombo, Alain, Söderlund & Khuu a paper about SDAM.  Since the paper's publication, hundreds of people approached Dr Levine thinking they too have SDAM.  Levin's group now tests these people.  Levine assumes he'll find less than 15 people with SDAM in this group.

The three people with SDAM are intelligent people (their IQs are above 110), they are all well educated (one of them has a Phd.) and they all work. Their poor autobiographical memory does not affect their everyday functioning.  These people are healthy, they do not suffer head injuries, neurological diseases or psychological trauma.  Two of them have had a single episode of depression many years ago, when they were adolescents.  Two out of three are in long term relationships and one is single.

All of them realized they have poor autobiographical memory during late adolescence or early adulthood. 

The researchers compared them with 15 people similar in age (47 on average) and education (16.5 years of education on average). 

Of the cognitive tests, the only measure all three were poor at was recall of the RCFT figure.      







The authors think that visual recall is inherent to autobiographical remembering.  That is, when we recall a past event we create a visual image of it.  They cite another study in which two healthy people with a total inability to form visual imagery throughout their lives had poor ability to relive episodic events.  One of them, who by chance had neuropsychological testing in the past, had poor visual memory, and poor RCFT performance. 

How was autobiographical memory assessed in this study?

Each person of the SDAM and the control group chose two events from each of six life periods:  a week ago, a month ago, a year ago, ten years ago, during adolescence and in childhood. Each of these events had to have happened at a specific time and place.  The people could look at almanacs or ask people close to them to name such events (for instance, "on July 2016 you've visited Seorak Park in South Korea").  The consulted people were asked not to add any details about the event.

Each person of the SDAM and the control group elaborated about these events to the best of his ability.  The researchers analyzed the stories and classified the details to be episodic (directly related to the main event, specific to time and space, creates a feeling of re-experience) or semantic/"external" (for example, facts about Korea of about other places in Korea which are not necessarily related to the same visit). 

The researchers found that the people with SDAM produced much less episodic details relative to the control group – but only for events that happened during youth and adolescence. 

The number of episodic details that the three people with SDAM could tell about events in later lifetimes was not significantly different than that of the control group.  This was so despite the fact that SDAM people said they don't have an experience of these events.  How could this be?

The researchers assume that SDAM people could provide episodic-like details as a result of activating non episodic processes (semantic processes).  SDAM people are used to make up for the lack of personal experience by becoming familiar with the story of the event.  The story of the event is retold each time the event is mentioned, and people who were present when the event took place have a chance to add details to the retelling.  Pictures from the event also enrich the story.  I'll also hypothesize that events from the remote past are less often spontaneously mentioned, and are mentioned less in the company of people who took part in them and can enrich the story.

Apparently it's hard to distinguish between episodic details and episodic-like details.  Often "rememberer" himself has difficulty distinguishing between them.  Each time we tell an episodic event we combine episodic details and episodic-like details.  I'm not sure that we can distinguish between small details that we remember directly from an event that occurred many years ago, and small details that someone else who was present in the event added to the event's story.  During remembering we are also assisted, without being aware of it, by a "script" or a "schema" according to which such an event is supposed to happen.

This emphasizes the extent to which autobiographical memory is a phenomenon that is in between the personal and the interpersonal/social.  Autobiographical memory is to some extent a story with a number of authors, a story that is influenced by values and norms of the culture we live in.

In order to overcome such issues the authors let judges who were blind to the person's group affiliation have a qualitative impression of the memory.  The judges did get the impression that the memory quality of the three SDAM people for events that occurred not only during childhood and adolescence, but also ten years or one year or one week before was poorer (in terms of the richness of the memory and the extent of reference to events that happened before and after the main event) than the memory quality of the control group.
Neurologically, the three SDAM people had smaller right hippocampuses than the control group.  During recall of episodic events under fMRI, SDAM people had less activity in brain areas related to autobiographical memory and visual memory.

To sum up, we've seen evidence for the possible existence of a group of people who can't remember events from their lives, who have no episodic memory.  These are healthy, educated working people that are highly functioning cognitively and interpersonally.  Due to the small size of the group it's hard to tell whether their poor visual memory characterizes other people with SDAM.  Generally, it's hard to come to any conclusions on the basis of a group of three people.  A larger study is being currently conducted.

These findings are thought provoking.  We are used to think about our life story as an essential part of our selves, and of autobiographical memory as an essential part of our selves.  But these people definitely have a self concept, personality, hobbies, values, beliefs, opinions and friends.  They know who they are.

Furthermore: the ability of these people to function so well in everyday life in all respects – professionally, personally – raise the question: to what extent is episodic memory essential? And if it's not essential – why did it develop?

       
Palombo, D. J., Alain, C., Söderlund, H., Khuu, W., & Levine, B. (2015). Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: A new mnemonic syndromeNeuropsychologia72, 105-118. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S002839321500158X

The strange case of the woman who couldn't remember her past – and can't imagine her future.  Erika Hayasaki.  WIRED.

No comments:

Post a Comment